| Home | E-Submission | Sitemap | Contact Us |  
top_img
Cancer Research and Treatment > Volume 47(2); 2015 > Article
Kye, Park, Oh, and Park: Perceptions of Cancer Risk and Cause of Cancer Risk in Korean Adults

Abstract

Purpose

The aims of the present study were to assess the prevalence of perceived risk for cancer; to explore associations between sociodemographics and family history of cancer and perceived cancer risk; to identify perceived cause of cancer risk; and to examine the associations between sociodemographics and family history of cancer and perceived cause of cancer risk.

Materials and Methods

This cross-sectional study was conducted among 1,009 participants aged 30-69 years, selected from a population-based database in October 2009 through multiple-stratified random sampling. Information was collected about the participants’ perceived cancer risk and perceived cause of cancer risk.

Results

Overall, 59.5% of the respondents thought they had the chance of developing cancer. Female sex, younger age, lower income, and family history of cancer were positively associated with perceived cancer risk. The most important perceived cause of cancer risk was stress. There was a difference between sociodemographics and family history of cancer and perceived cause of cancer risk.

Conclusion

Factors affecting perceptions of cancer risk and cause of cancer risk need to be addressed in risk communications. The results provide important directions for the development of educational strategies to promote awareness and self-appraisal of cancer risk and risk factors.

Introduction

Individuals possess cognitive representations of various diseases [1]. These representations consist of beliefs about the causes, symptoms, timeline, consequences, and treatment of a given disease. Importantly, the set of beliefs subsumed by the representation motivates, guides, and shapes an individual’s health-related behavior [2]. Beliefs about a particular disease can influence whether and how an individual takes action to reduce the risk for developing that disease. Consequently, the study of these disease-related be liefs is important for the development of interventions targeting risk-reducing health behavior.
Perceived risk is defined as the subjective belief about the likelihood or probability of harm, that is, the probability that a health problem will be experienced if no precautions or behavioral changes occur [3-5]. Perceived risk has been identified as part of the “motivational engine” behind many health-protective actions. Individuals who feel at lower-than-average risk of a disease are less likely to engage in preventive behavior [5-8]. In relation to the perceived risk for cancer, a higher proportion of people appear to believe that they are at lower than average risk, therefore, it is important to understand the determinants of perceived risk [9].
Several studies have examined perceived risk for cancer and its associated factors [6,7,10-13]. Previous studies have explored demographic, health status, lifestyle, and psychosocial factors associated with perceived risk for breast or colorectal cancer. For example, a population-based study found that higher perceived risk for colorectal cancer was associated with a family history of colorectal cancer, poorer subjective health, more bowel symptoms, higher anxiety, smoking, and not exercising, whereas being male and older were associated with lower perceived risk [10]. These studies have predominantly focused on breast and colorectal cancer and have rarely measured perceived risk in Asians. There were few earlier studies on the correlates of perceived risk for general cancer using a population-based sample.
In the present study, we included sociodemographic factors hypothesized to influence perceived risk, and asked about the reasons for risk judgments. Many studies of attributions of cancer risk have focused on the five groups of determinants first described by Weinstein [7,12,14-16]: actions and behavior patterns (e.g., diet, exercise, smoking, drinking, going for checkups); heredity (e.g., family history); physiological attributes (e.g., age, current perceived health, and health problems); psychological attributes (e.g., stress, being an optimist/pessimist); and environmental factors (e.g., pollution, occupational exposure). Lipkus et al. [12] concluded that the majority of their predominantly African-American sample attributed their cancer risk to psychological factors (35%). This was followed by heredity (20%), personal actions (17%), and physiological factors (12%), with few citing environmental reasons (0.005%). Blalock et al. [15] found that among a sample of adults with a first-degree relative with colorectal cancer, physiology was mentioned most frequently (27%), closely followed by heredity (25%) and personal actions (16%) as determinants of risk, whereas first-degree relatives of surgical patients cited personal actions and physiology with equal frequency (27%) and heredity less often (10%). Robb et al. [7] reported that among adults from the patient lists of two general practices, the most frequently cited reasons for cancer risk were diet, family history, and symptoms/general health. In contrast to the results of Lipkus et al. [12], psychological factors were not mentioned with sufficient frequency for statistical analysis. The difference could be related to ethnicity; the participants in the study by Lipkus et al. [12] were predominantly African-American and had lower sociodemographic status, whereas those of Blalock et al. [15] and Robb et al. [7] were predominantly white, but difference in family history could have played a part. Few previous studies have examined attribution of perceived risk in a population-based sample.
The aims of the present study were to assess the prevalence of perceived risk for cancer; to explore associations between sociodemographics and family history of cancer and perceived cancer risk; to identify perceived cause of cancer risk; and to examine the associations between sociodemographics and family history of cancer and perceived cause of cancer risk. On the basis of previous research, we predicted that higher perceived risk for cancer would be associated with a family history of cancer, whereas being male and older would be associated with lower perceived risk; and that there would be a difference between sociodemographics and family history of cancer and perceived cause of cancer risk.

Materials and Methods

1. Design and sample

The participants for this cross-sectional study were chosen from a population-based database through multiplestratified random sampling. A total of 1,009 participants aged 30-69 years with no history of cancer were engaged in face-to-face interviews by investigators from a professional research agency in October 2009. Information was collected about the participants’ sex, age, educational level, monthly income, job, residential area, family history of cancer, perceived cancer risk, and perceived cause of cancer risk. The study was approved by the Institute of Review Board at the Korean National Cancer Center, and informed written consent was obtained from all study participants.

2. Measures

1) Perceived cancer risk

To measure perceived cancer risk, each participant was asked to respond ‘yes’ or ‘no’ to the following question: ‘Do you think that you have a chance of developing cancer?’

2) Perceived cause of cancer risk

Among participants who responded yes to the above question about perceived cancer risk, cause of cancer risk was assessed with a single item: ‘What do you think is the cause of cancer risk if you have a chance of developing cancer?’ Respondents were encouraged to list three risk factors in seven predefined categories. These included smoking, alcohol consumption, unhealthy diet, physical inactivity, stress, heredity, and environmental pollution.

3) Analysis

The χ2 test was used to assess the relationships between sociodemographics and family history of cancer and perceived cancer risk. Binary logistic regression was subsequently performed, with perceived cancer risk as the dependent variable and all individual characteristics as independent variables. To examine the relationship between sociodemographics and family history of cancer and perceived cause of cancer risk, binary logistic regression analysis was conducted, with perceived cause of cancer risk as the dependent variable and all individual characteristics as independent variables. Data were analyzed using SPSS ver. 15.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL).

Results

Table 1 shows the characteristics of the study population. Among the respondents, 49.5% were male and 42.1% had a college-level education, whereas 15.1% had not completed high school. Nearly two-thirds of the participants were employed and 27.0% had a family history of cancer.
Perceived cancer risk according to the sociodemographics and family history of cancer is summarized in Table 2. Overall, 59.5% of the respondents thought they had the chance of developing cancer. Significant bivariate differences in perceived cancer risk were observed for sex, age, education, monthly income, job, and family history of cancer. These variables were entered into a multivariate logistic model. Women were more likely to perceive cancer risk than men (odds ratio [OR], 1.39). Younger age and lower income were associated with greater perception of cancer risk (OR, 0.68 to 0.69 and 0.73, respectively). Family history of cancer was associated with perceived cancer risk (OR, 2.80).
Table 3 shows the perceived cause of cancer risk according to the sociodemographics and family history of cancer. Taken as a whole, respondents thought that the most important cause of cancer risk to themselves was stress, followed by unhealthy diet, smoking, alcohol consumption, physical inactivity, heredity, and environmental pollution. Family history of cancer was a significant predictor of identifying any of the perceived causes of cancer risk, except physical inactivity (OR, 1.53 to 2.67). Women were less likely to perceive that smoking or alcohol consumption was the cause of cancer risk than men were (OR, 0.23 to 0.28). With regard to unhealthy diet, higher income was associated with greater perception of cause of cancer risk (OR, 1.46 and 1.59, respectively). Respondents aged 40-59 years were more likely to perceive that smoking was a cause of cancer risk than those aged 20-39 years (OR, 1.63). As age increased, respondents perceived more that alcohol consumption was a cause of cancer risk (OR, 1.71 and 1.93). Respondents residing in a rural area were less likely to perceive that alcohol consumption was a cause of cancer risk (OR, 0.52). Respondents living in a smaller area, had a greater perception that physical inactivity was a cause of cancer risk (OR, 1.84 and 2.59). Respondents who earned > 5,000 dollars a month were more likely to perceive that heredity was a cause of cancer risk (OR, 2.30). Respondents residing in a small-medium–sized city were less likely to perceive heredity as a cause of cancer risk, and at the same time, were more likely to perceive environmental pollution as a cause (OR, 0.54 and 2.69, respectively). With regard to job status, students were less likely to perceive environmental pollution as a cause of cancer risk than those who had no job (OR, 0.25).

Discussion

The current study used a national representative sample to assess the prevalence of perceived risk of cancer, to explore associations between sociodemographics and family history of cancer and perceived cancer risk; to identify perceived cause of cancer risk; and to examine the associations between sociodemographics and family history of cancer and perceived cause of cancer risk. We found that respondents who were female, aged 20-39 years, had low income, and a family history of cancer viewed their cancer risk as higher than that of their peers. Stress was the most frequently mentioned as the perceived cause of cancer risk, followed by unhealthy diet, smoking, alcohol consumption, physical inactivity, heredity, and environmental pollution, and there were diverse relationships between health behavior and perceived cause of cancer risk.
It was interesting to find that being male and older were both associated with lower perceived cancer risk, because these two factors have consistently been linked to higher risk of cancer. These findings highlight the need for future risk communications to address any misperceptions surrounding age and sex. The inverse relationship between age and perceived risk is consistent with a previous study using a representative female sample in the United Kingdom [17], in which 35% of those aged > 65 years reported reduced perception of personal risk of breast cancer in comparison to the general population, which is higher than the average of 17%. This may reflect existing evidence regarding participants’ barriers, including poor knowledge and lack of awareness of age-related breast cancer risk among older women [17]. For example, only 30% of women knew that advanced age is a risk factor for developing breast cancer and older women were less able to identify risk factors correctly [17].
In line with a previous study [18], another noteworthy finding was a link between income level and perceived cancer risk. Socioeconomically disadvantaged persons may feel more likely to be exposed to certain environmental hazards or have lower literacy skills, thus increasing their sense of vulnerability. There is a growing body of evidence that social position is an important factor that drives the widening disparities in cancer outcomes [19]. Further research on the interaction of risk perceptions and cancer preventive behavior among minorities may prove useful.
There was one less surprising finding. Family history of cancer was the most influential determinant of perceived cancer risk, consistent with previous studies [10,11,13,18]. This result might indicate that participants are aware of the objective medical and environmental effects of family history of cancer. At the same time, however, this finding is of some concern. Several studies showed that overestimation of cancer risk and associated heightened psychological distress have been documented among individuals with a family history of cancer [20,21]. The problem with individuals using family history of cancer to judge their own risk is that the genetic link for most cancer is limited. Given the fact that genetics plays a relatively small part in cancer causation compared to lifestyle factors [22], the public does not seem to have sufficient knowledge about cancer, especially in genetics [23]. Thus, this study may suggest future efforts to maintain further assessment and interventions to promote accurate understanding of cancer risk.
Somewhat striking was the high attribution of risk placed on relatively uncontrollable factors, such as stress. The most frequently mentioned perceived cause of cancer was stress, followed by unhealthy diet, smoking, alcohol consumption, physical inactivity, heredity, and environmental pollution. Kristeller et al. [24] found that cancer patients and their relatives cited stress as the most important cause of their cancer, followed by bad luck, heredity, and environmental pollution, and, to a lesser extent, modifiable risk factors, such as diet and alcohol. Attributing higher risk to external or uncontrollable factors than to personal behavior supports the “defensiveness” hypothesis. Such a belief pattern may present a barrier to concerted efforts for behavior change.
With regard to the associations between sociodemographics and family history of cancer and perceived cause of cancer risk, there was an adverse pattern between health-promoting and health-threatening behavior. Specifically, in the health-promoting behavior such as diet and physical activity, the more that people indulged in healthy behavior, the more sensitive to cause of cancer risk they became. Individuals with a higher income were more likely to perceive unhealthy diet as a cause of cancer. Kye et al. [25] revealed that respondents with a higher income consumed a healthy diet more often. Also, people who lived in a small-medium–sized city or rural area where manual labor that requires heavy work was common were more likely to perceive physical inactivity as a cause of cancer. In contrast, in the healththreatening behaviors such as smoking, alcohol consumption, and pollution, the more that people engaged in unhealthy behavior, the less concerned they were about cause of cancer risk. In the present study, men were more likely to perceive smoking and alcohol consumption as causes of cancer. It is well known that men have a higher prevalence of smoking and alcohol consumption than women have. Finally, except for physical inactivity, all respondents mentioned a family history of cancer as a determinant of perceived risk. As mentioned above, excessive worry about family history of cancer could lead to unhealthy behavior, such as avoidance of cancer screening, thus further intervention to help people achieve an appropriate perception of cancer risk is necessary.
In interpreting the results, it is appropriate to consider several limitations. First, because data for this study were cross-sectional, it is beyond the scope of this research to establish causality. Longitudinal studies would be needed to track changes in risk perceptions associated with changes in cancer prevention practices. Second, the data for this study lacked information on risk perceptions by cancer site, thereby undermining precision in guiding educational strategies for specific types of cancer. Cancer is a set of heterogeneous diseases. Perceived cancer risk could be different by cancer type. Previous studies have mainly focused on breast cancer, cervical cancer, colorectal cancer, or skin cancer, respectively. More studies are needed to identify the difference of perceived cancer risk by cancer site in a single target. However, despite these limitations, we identified the prevalence of perceived risk for cancer, explored associations between sociodemographics and family history of cancer and perceived cancer risk, identified perceived cause of cancer risk, and examined the associations between sociodemographics and family history of cancer and perceived cause of cancer risk using a national representative sample. These results will provide important directions for the development of educational strategies to promote awareness and self-appraisal of cancer risk.

Conclusion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate factors associated with perceived risk for general cancer and cause of cancer risk in a population sample in an Asian country. Several factors tended to be associated with lower levels of perceived cancer risk: male sex, older age, higher income, and no family history of cancer. The most important perceived cause of cancer was stress. These results may serve as barriers to preventive health behavior. In general, people who believe themselves to be at lower risk than others for an adverse health outcome and attribute risk to uncontrollable factors are less enthusiastic about engaging in healthy preventative behavior such as screening. In practical terms, the evaluation of perceived risk for cancer and perceived cause of cancer risk may be useful to clinicians in recommending screening tests and incorporating an intervention to educate people about the actual risk and risk factors. The results from this survey highlight the need for health communication and education aimed at increasing the ability of individuals in Korea to perceive their personal risk for cancer and risk factors.

Conflicts of Interest

Conflict of interest relevant to this article was not reported.

Acknowledgments

This study was financially supported by National Cancer Center Grant 1310260-2. No potential conflicts of interest were disclosed.

Table 1.
Characteristics of study population
No. (%) (n=1,009)
Gender
 Male 499 (49.5)
 Female 510 (50.5)
Age (yr)
 20-39 429 (42.5)
 40-59 393 (38.9)
 ≥ 60 187 (18.6)
Education
 Middle school or lower 152 (15.1)
 High school 432 (42.8)
 College graduate 425 (42.1)
Monthly income ($)
 < 2,999 358 (35.5)
 3,000-4,999 480 (47.6)
 ≥ 5,000 165 (16.4)
 No response 6 (0.5)
Job
 None 39 (3.9)
 Blue color 435 (43.1)
 White color 251 (24.9)
 Housewife 213 (21.1)
 Student 67 (6.6)
 No response 4 (0.4)
Residential area
 Metropolitan 479 (47.5)
 Small-medium city 350 (34.7)
 Rural 180 (17.8)
Family history of cancer
 No 737 (73.0)
 Yes 272 (27.0)
Table 2.
Perceived cancer risk according to sociodemographics and family history of cancern
Bivariate analysis
Multivariate analysis
Yes No χ2 (p-value) OR 95% CI
Total 600 (59.5) 409 (40.5)
Gender
 Male 280 (56.1) 219 (43.9) 4.906 1.00
 Female 320 (62.7) 190 (37.3) (0.027) 1.39 1.17-1.55
Age (yr)
 20-39 286 (66.7) 143 (33.3) 20.366 1.00
 40-59 212 (53.9) 181 (46.1) (< 0.001) 0.69 0.54-0.93
 ≥ 60 102 (54.5) 85 (45.5) 0.68 0.41-0.92
Education
 Middle school or lower 100 (65.8) 52 (34.2) 7.316 1.00
 High school 246 (56.9) 186 (43.1) (0.026) 0.81 0.71-1.13
 College graduate 254 (59.8) 171 (40.2) 0.83 0.73-1.17
Monthly income ($)
 < 2,999 234 (65.4) 124 (34.6) 8.669 1.00
 3,000-4,999 273 (56.9) 207 (43.1) (0.013) 0.87 0.62-1.11
 ≥ 5,000 90 (54.5) 75 (45.5) 0.73 0.61-0.92
Job
 None 19 (48.7) 20 (51.3) 26.503 1.00
 Blue color 254 (58.4) 181 (41.6) (< 0.001) 1.18 0.57-2.45
 White color 168 (66.9) 83 (33.1) 1.55 0.70-3.41
 Housewife 134 (62.9) 79 (37.1) 1.75 0.81-3.80
 Student 23 (34.2) 44 (65.8) 0.56 0.22-1.40
Residential area
 Metropolitan 288 (60.1) 191 (39.9) 0.197 1.00
 Small-medium city 207 (59.1) 143 (40.9) (0.906) 1.10 0.81-1.49
 Rural 105 (58.3) 75 (41.7) 1.15 0.74-1.77
Family history of cancer
 No 391 (53.1) 346 (46.9) 46.631 1.00
 Yes 209 (76.8) 63 (23.2) (< 0.001) 2.80 2.01-3.91

CI, confidence interval.

Table 3.
Odds ratios of perceived cause of cancer risk from logistic regression of sociodemographics and family history of cancer
Stress (n=377,62.8%) Unhealthy diet (n=319,53.2%) Smoking (n=264,44.0%) Drinking (n=204, 34.0%) Physical inactivity (n=188,31.3%) Heredity (n=179,29.8%) Environmental pollution (n=134,22.3%)
Gender
 Male 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
 Female 0.84 (0.61-1.15) 1.17 (0.84-1.62) 0.28 (0.19-0.40)*** 0.23 (0.14-0.35)*** 1.15 (0.77-1.72) 1.25 (0.83-1.87) 1.12 (0.70-1.80)
Age (yr)
 20-39 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
 40-59 1.22 (0.87-1.71) 1.26 (0.89-1.79) 1.63 (1.11-2.38)* 1.71 (1.14-2.58)* 1.11 (0.73-1.69) 1.05 (0.68-1.60) 0.64 (0.40-1.03)
 ≥ 60 0.78 (0.46-1.31) 0.97 (0.56-1.66) 1.41 (0.78-2.53) 1.93 (1.02-3.67)* 0.96 (0.50-1.86) 1.10 (0.58-2.11) 0.54 (0.26-1.13)
Education 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
Middle school or lower
 High school 1.48 (0.86-2.55) 1.21 (0.70-2.10) 0.72(0.40-1.31) 1.44 (0.72-2.88) 1.94 (0.98-3.86) 0.94 (0.48-1.85) 0.89 (0.41-1.92)
 College graduate 1.23 (0.65-2.29) 1.44 (0.76-2.73) 0.72 (0.36-1.44) 1.42 (0.64-3.18) 2.07 (0.93-4.57) 0.97 (0.44-2.13) 0.64 (0.27-1.56)
Monthly income ($)
 < 2,999 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
 3,000-4,999 1.15 (0.84-1.58) 1.46 (1.05-2.04)* 1.20 (0.83-1.74) 1.02 (0.68-1.52) 0.75 (0.51-1.11) 1.32 (0.86-2.02) 1.26 (0.80-2.00)
 ≥ 5,000 0.99 (0.64-1.52) 1.59 (1.02-2.48)* 1.48 (0.90-2.41) 1.27(0.75-2.13) 1.01 (0.60-1.70) 2.30 (1.36-3.90)** 1.59 (0.87-2.90)
Job
 None 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
 Blue color 0.86 (0.39-1.87) 0.61 (0.28-1.30) 1.46 (0.63-3.41) 1.82 (0.67-4.93) 1.21 (0.40-3.66) 1.13 (0.40-3.22) 0.45 (0.17-1.18)
 White color 1.42 (0.63-3.23) 0.64 (0.28-1.43) 1.74 (0.70-4.34) 1.82 (0.63-5.27) 1.52 (0.48-4.77) 1.37 (0.46-4.11) 0.36 (0.13-1.03)
 Housewife 1.18 (0.52-2.66) 0.97 (0.44-2.15) 0.86 (0.33-2.24) 1.07 (0.34-3.38) 2.55 (0.82-7.94) 1.40 (0.48-4.08) 0.90 (0.34-2.45)
 Student 0.66 (0.24-1.76) 0.38 (0.14-1.04) 0.63 (0.19-2.05) 0.82 (0.21-3.08) 0.75 (0.19-2.96) 0.84 (0.22-3.02) 0.25 (0.07-0.94)*
Residential area 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
Metropolitan
 Small-medium city 0.99 (0.73-1.34) 0.88 (0.64-1.20) 0.71 (0.49-1.01) 0.69 (0.47-1.02) 1.84 (1.25-2.68)** 0.54 (0.36-0.81)** 2.69 (1.77-4.08)***
 Rural 1.03 (0.67-1.58) 1.09 (0.70-1.70) 0.89 (0.55-1.43) 0.52 (0.30-0.89)* 2.59 (1.55-4.34)*** 0.63 (0.36-1.11) 0.75 (0.35-1.62)
Family history of
cancer
 No 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
 Yes 1.87 (1.39-2.51)*** 1.56 (1.15-2.11)** 1.59 (1.13-2.23)** 1.54 (1.07-2.23)* 1.12 (0.77-1.61) 2.67 (1.87-3.81)*** 1.53 (1.01-2.32)*

* p < 0.05,

** p < 0.01,

*** p < 0.001.

References

1. Lau RR, Hartmann KA. Common sense representations of common illnesses. Health Psychol. 1983;2:167–85.
crossref
2. Leventhal H, Diefenbach M, Leventhal EA. Illness cognition: using common sense to understand treatment adherence and affect cognition interactions. Cognit Ther Res. 1992;16:143–63.
crossref
3. Glanz K, Lewis FM, Rimer BK. Health behavior and health education: theory, research, and practice. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers; 1990.

4. Katapodi MC, Lee KA, Facione NC, Dodd MJ. Predictors of perceived breast cancer risk and the relation between perceived risk and breast cancer screening: a meta-analytic review. Prev Med. 2004;38:388–402.
crossref pmid
5. Kwak MS, Choi KS, Park S, Park EC. Perceived risk for gastric cancer among the general Korean population: a populationbased survey. Psychooncology. 2009;18:708–15.
crossref pmid
6. McCaul KD, Branstetter AD, Schroeder DM, Glasgow RE. What is the relationship between breast cancer risk and mammography screening? A meta-analytic review. Health Psychol. 1996;15:423–9.
crossref pmid
7. Robb KA, Miles A, Wardle J. Perceived risk of colorectal cancer: sources of risk judgments. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2007;16:694–702.
crossref pmid
8. Zhang LR, Chiarelli AM, Glendon G, Mirea L, Edwards S, Knight JA, et al. Influence of perceived breast cancer risk on screening behaviors of female relatives from the Ontario site of the Breast Cancer Family Registry. Eur J Cancer Prev. 2011;20:255–62.
crossref pmid pmc
9. Weinstein ND. Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: conclusions from a community-wide sample. J Behav Med. 1987;10:481–500.
crossref pmid
10. Robb KA, Miles A, Wardle J. Demographic and psychosocial factors associated with perceived risk for colorectal cancer. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2004;13:366–72.
crossref pmid pdf
11. Vernon SW, Myers RE, Tilley BC, Li S. Factors associated with perceived risk in automotive employees at increased risk of colorectal cancer. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2001;10:35–43.
pmid
12. Lipkus IM, Rimer BK, Lyna PR, Pradhan AA, Conaway M, Woods-Powell CT. Colorectal screening patterns and perceptions of risk among African-American users of a community health center. J Community Health. 1996;21:409–27.
crossref pmid
13. Vernon SW, Vogel VG, Halabi S, Bondy ML. Factors associated with perceived risk of breast cancer among women attending a screening program. Breast Cancer Res Treat. 1993;28:137–44.
crossref pmid
14. Weinstein ND. Why it won't happen to me: perceptions of risk factors and susceptibility. Health Psychol. 1984;3:431–57.
crossref pmid
15. Blalock SJ, DeVellis BM, Afifi RA, Sandler RS. Risk perceptions and participation in colorectal cancer screening. Health Psychol. 1990;9:792–806.
crossref pmid
16. Lipkus IM, Skinner CS, Green LS, Dement J, Samsa GP, Ransohoff D. Modifying attributions of colorectal cancer risk. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2004;13:560–6.
crossref pmid pdf
17. Grunfeld EA, Ramirez AJ, Hunter MS, Richards MA. Women's knowledge and beliefs regarding breast cancer. Br J Cancer. 2002;86:1373–8.
crossref pmid pmc
18. Honda K, Neugut AI. Associations between perceived cancer risk and established risk factors in a national community sample. Cancer Detect Prev. 2004;28:1–7.
crossref pmid
19. Krieger N, Quesenberry C Jr, Peng T, Horn-Ross P, Stewart S, Brown S, et al. Social class, race/ethnicity, and incidence of breast, cervix, colon, lung, and prostate cancer among Asian, Black, Hispanic, and White residents of the San Francisco Bay Area, 1988-92 (United States). Cancer Causes Control. 1999;10:525–37.
crossref pmid
20. Bluman LG, Rimer BK, Berry DA, Borstelmann N, Iglehart JD, Regan K, et al. Attitudes, knowledge, and risk perceptions of women with breast and/or ovarian cancer considering testing for BRCA1 and BRCA2. J Clin Oncol. 1999;17:1040–6.
crossref pmid
21. Daly MB, Lerman CL, Ross E, Schwartz MD, Sands CB, Masny A. Gail model breast cancer risk components are poor predictors of risk perception and screening behavior. Breast Cancer Res Treat. 1996;41:59–70.
crossref pmid
22. Ahlbom A, Lichtenstein P, Malmstrom H, Feychting M, Hemminki K, Pedersen NL. Cancer in twins: genetic and nongenetic familial risk factors. J Natl Cancer Inst. 1997;89:287–93.
crossref pmid
23. Shin KR, Park HJ, Kim M. Practice of breast self-examination and knowledge of breast cancer among female university students in Korea. Nurs Health Sci. 2012;14:292–7.
crossref pmid
24. Kristeller JL, Hebert J, Edmiston K, Liepman M, Wertheimer M, Ward A, et al. Attitudes toward risk factor behavior of relatives of cancer patients. Prev Med. 1996;25:162–9.
crossref pmid
25. Kye SY, Yun EH, Park K. Factors related to self-perception of diet quality among South Korean adults. Asian Pac J Cancer Prev. 2012;13:1495–504.
crossref pmid
Editorial Office
Korean Cancer Association
Room 1824, Gwanghwamun Officia
92 Saemunan-ro, Jongno-gu, Seoul 03186, Korea
TEL: +82-2-3276-2410   FAX: +82-2-792-1410   E-mail: journal@cancer.or.kr
About |  Browse Articles |  Current Issue |  For Authors and Reviewers
Copyright © Korean Cancer Association.                 Developed in M2PI